| |||||||||||
SI IOSRT 2023 : Special Issue on Integration of Observational Studies with Randomized Trials | |||||||||||
Link: https://www.degruyter.com/publication/journal_key/JCI/downloadAsset/JCI_CFP%20Integration%20of%20Observational%20Studies%20with%20Randomized%20Trials.pdf | |||||||||||
| |||||||||||
Call For Papers | |||||||||||
SPECIAL ISSUE on Integration of Observational Studies with Randomized Trials
JOURNAL OF CAUSAL INFERENCE https://www.degruyter.com/journal/key/jci/html COORDINATING EDITORS Elias Bareinboim, Department of Computer Science, Columbia University Mark van der Laan, Division of Biostatistics, School of Public Health UC Berkeley SUBMISSION DEADLINE The deadline for submissions is DECEMBER 15, 2022, but individual papers will be reviewed and published online on an ongoing basis CONTACT jci_editorial@degruyter.com DESCRIPTION Designs for causal inferences can be thought of as existing on a spectrum. On the one extreme are tightly controlled experimental studies, in which adherence to ideal randomly assigned treatment is optimized. Such studies minimize challenges to causal identification arising from confounding, but may be susceptible to sample selection bias and other imperfections. In addition, due both to cost and intensity of effort, sample sizes for such studies may be limited. On the other extreme are observational studies, in which data on more comprehensive, representative of real world conditions and larger samples are available. In such studies, however, confounding often poses challenges to causal identification. Both extremes, and especially randomized trials, require transfer techniques when findings are transported to new populations or new environments. The possibility of using one to alleviate weaknesses of the other has surely occurred to the first generation of statisticians who were confronting the design of experiments. However, lacking a formal language of causal analysis, such possibilities have remained in the realm of informal folklore, or rules of thumb, and have not impacted data analytic practice until recently. The landscape has changed since the introduction of formal causal models (Rubin 1974, Robin and Greenland, 1986, Pearl 2000) especially through the formal definitions of notions such as "causal effects", "confounding" and "counterfactuals". Clever combinations of experimental and observational studies have been shown capable of yielding gains in several aspects of causal analysis; example are provided below: 1. Identification The theory of transportability and data fusion (Bareinboim and Pearl 2016) has shown how data from diverse sources, both experimental and observational, can be combined to identify causal quantities which are not estimable with of each source in isolation. 2. Explanation and Probabilities of causation The combination of experimental and observational studies were shown capable of yielding informative bounds on probabilities of sufficiency (PS) and probabilities of necessity (PN), both essential in producing plausible explanations, in the assessment of "Causes of Effects" and in inferring individual behavior from population data. (Tian and Pearl, 2001; Pearl 2015, R-431). 3. Detection of experimental imperfection The combination of experimental and observational studies were shown capable for detecting experimental imperfections such as selection bias and latent heterogeneity. 4. Variance reduction Observational studies on certain aspects of a population were shown capable of reducing the variance associated with effect estimates in experimental studies (Pearl, 2018). This reduction is especially useful in the presence of selection bias (Rosenman et al 2021). |
|